Encyc

Encyc houses over 100 concepts relevant to the history of eugenics and its continued implications in contemporary life. These entries represent in-depth explorations of key concepts for understanding eugenics.

Aboriginal and Indigenous Peoples
Michael Billinger
Alcoholism and drug use
Paula Larsson
Archives and institutions
Mary Horodyski
Assimilation
Karen Stote
Bioethical appeals to eugenics
Tiffany Campbell
Bioethics
Gregor Wolbring
Birth control
Molly Ladd-Taylor
Childhood innocence
Joanne Faulkner
Colonialism
Karen Stote
Conservationism
Michael Kohlman
Criminality
Amy Samson
Degeneracy
Michael Billinger
Dehumanization: psychological aspects
David Livingstone Smith
Deinstitutionalization
Erika Dyck
Developmental disability
Dick Sobsey
Disability rights
Joshua St. Pierre
Disability, models of
Gregor Wolbring
Down Syndrome
Michael Berube
Education
Erna Kurbegovic
Education as redress
Jonathan Chernoguz
Educational testing
Michelle Hawks
Environmentalism
Douglas Wahlsten
Epilepsy
Frank W. Stahnisch
Ethnicity and race
Michael Billinger
Eugenic family studies
Robert A. Wilson
Eugenic traits
Robert A. Wilson
Eugenics
Robert A. Wilson
Eugenics as wrongful
Robert A. Wilson
Eugenics: positive vs negative
Robert A. Wilson
Family planning
Caroline Lyster
Farming and animal breeding
Sheila Rae Gibbons
Feeble-mindedness
Wendy Kline
Feminism
Esther Rosario
Fitter family contests
Molly Ladd-Taylor
Gender
Caroline Lyster
Genealogy
Leslie Baker
Genetic counseling
Gregor Wolbring
Genetics
James Tabery
Genocide
Karen Stote
Guidance clinics
Amy Samson
Hereditary disease
Sarah Malanowski
Heredity
Michael Billinger
Human enhancement
Gregor Wolbring
Human experimentation
Frank W. Stahnisch
Human nature
Chris Haufe
Huntington's disease
Alice Wexler
Immigration
Jacalyn Ambler
Indian--race-based definition
Karen Stote
Informed consent
Erika Dyck
Institutionalization
Erika Dyck
Intellectual disability
Licia Carlson
Intelligence and IQ testing
Aida Roige
KEY CONCEPTS
Robert A. Wilson
Kant on eugenics and human nature
Alan McLuckie
Marriage
Alexandra Minna Stern
Masturbation
Paula Larsson
Medicalization
Gregor Wolbring
Mental deficiency: idiot, imbecile, and moron
Wendy Kline
Miscegenation
Michael Billinger
Motherhood
Molly Ladd-Taylor
Natural and artificial selection
Douglas Wahlsten
Natural kinds
Matthew H. Slater
Nature vs nurture
James Tabery
Nazi euthanasia
Paul Weindling
Nazi sterilization
Paul Weindling
Newgenics
Caroline Lyster
Nordicism
Michael Kohlman
Normalcy and subnormalcy
Gregor Wolbring
Parenting and newgenics
Caroline Lyster
Parenting of children with disabilities
Dick Sobsey
Parenting with intellectual disabilities
David McConnell
Pauperism
Caroline Lyster
Person
Gregor Wolbring
Physician assisted suicide
Caroline Lyster
Political science and race
Dexter Fergie
Popular culture
Colette Leung
Population control
Alexandra Stern
Prenatal testing
Douglas Wahlsten
Project Prevention
Samantha Balzer
Propaganda
Colette Leung
Psychiatric classification
Steeves Demazeux
Psychiatry and mental health
Frank W. Stahnisch
Psychology
Robert A. Wilson
Public health
Lindsey Grubbs
Race and racialism
Michael Billinger
Race betterment
Erna Kurbegovic
Race suicide
Adam Hochman
Racial hygiene
Frank W. Stahnisch
Racial hygiene and Nazism
Frank Stahnisch
Racial segregation
Paula Larsson
Racism
Michael Billinger
Reproductive rights
Erika Dyck
Reproductive technologies
Caroline Lyster
Residential schools
Faun Rice
Roles of science in eugenics
Robert A. Wilson
Schools for the Deaf and Deaf Identity
Bartlomiej Lenart
Science and values
Matthew J. Barker
Selecting for disability
Clarissa Becerra
Sexual segregation
Leslie Baker
Sexuality
Alexandra Minna Stern
Social Darwinism
Erna Kurbegovic
Sociobiology
Robert A. Wilson
Sorts of people
Robert A. Wilson
Special education
Jason Ellis
Speech-language pathology
Joshua St. Pierre
Standpoint theory
Joshua St. Pierre
Sterilization
Wendy Kline
Sterilization compensation
Paul Weindling
Stolen generations
Joanne Faulkner
Subhumanization
Licia Carlson
Today and Tomorrow: To-day and To-morrow book series
Michael Kohlman
Training schools for the feeble-minded
Katrina Jirik
Trans
Aleta Gruenewald
Transhumanism and radical enhancement
Mark Walker
Tuberculosis
Maureen Lux
Twin Studies
Douglas Wahlsten & Frank W. Stahnisch
Ugly Laws
Susan M. Schweik and Robert A. Wilson
Unfit, the
Cameron A.J. Ellis
Violence and disability
Dick Sobsey
War
Frank W. Stahnisch
Women's suffrage
Sheila Rae Gibbons

Intelligence and IQ testing

Intelligence, genius and mental ability were a cluster of traits that received much attention in eugenics discourse. Intelligence was regarded as one of the good qualities superior men possessed, in turn beneficial for society as a whole. On the other hand, the socially problematic or unproductive were identified as being of inferior mental quality: “feeble-minded”. By and large, eugenicists thought that (1) intelligence was a unitary psychological trait that could be measured, being quantified as an intelligence quotient (IQ); (2) intelligence was paired with educational achievement, reputation, and economic success; (3) a certain degree of intelligence was necessary to act morally and to foresee the consequences of one’s actions. Despite the prominent role of intelligence in the history of eugenics, there is considerable lack of clarity in what this general mental ability was considered to be.

Galton and the link to eugenics
For most of the history of eugenics, “intelligence” was used synonymously with “general mental ability”. In 1914, Stern wrote that intelligence could also be teleologically defined: it is what allows one to adapt and deal affectively to new problems and situations (p. 3). Today it is still common to refer to intelligence in these terms.

Although success in the conventional sense (occurring at the social level) cannot be solely attributed (reduced) to underlying psychological attributes, Francis Galton (1922-1911), who coined the term “eugenics”, thought that intelligence was one of the great traits that superior man possessed and contributed most to their success. Reasoning that intelligence was normally distributed within populations and correlated with eminence, he hypothesized in Hereditary Genius (1869) that this trait was heritable. Galton also unsuccessfully attempted to measure intellectual capacity by correlation with sensory and physical capacity (e.g. reaction times, gripping strength -- see 1890).

At the beginning of the twentieth century, psychology was moving towards the standardization of classifications and measurements. Influenced by Galton, there were other attempts to quantify intelligence in the Unite States, such as those of James M. Cattell (1890) and Charles Spearman (1904). Eugenics became increasingly popular, and with it attributing society’s problems (e.g. crime, poverty or promiscuity) to those thought to be “mentally defective” or “feeble-minded”. In North America, “feeblemindedness” was being clinically diagnosed mainly on grounds of teachers’ and parents’ impressions of a person’s abilities. Because such diagnoses are subject to hidden interests and biases, and given that those thought feebleminded were often subject to negative eugenic policies, an independent measure of feeblemindedness was sought.

Testing, from France to North America
In France, Alfred Binet (1857-1911) and collaborator Theodore Simon were motivated to develop a tool to identify children in need of alternative education, independent of history or genealogy. In 1908 they presented a test to determine educational placement. The test consisted of tasks of increasing complexity thought to reveal children’s abilities at various ages. A child’s score, given as a “mental age”, corresponded to the average age of children getting that child’s raw score. Differently from Galton, they though intelligence was mutable by way of training, mostly not inherited, and that higher-level processes were more central to it than sensorimotor ones –although earlier intelligence testing could have been over these merely because assessments were already available (Mackintosh, 2011b p.4).

The same year, the Binet-Simon scale was translated into English by Henry H. Goddard (1866-1957), superintendent at the Vineland Training School for Feeble-minded Boys and Girls, in New Jersey. He proposed to use individuals’ test scores to identify the “feeble-minded”. In 1910, Goddard introduced a hierarchy of feeble-mindedness that became the standard nomenclature for decades. It included the grades of “idiot” (mental age of up to 3 in adults), “imbecile” (3-7), as well as the new grade “moron” (8-12) for those who were only mildly mentally defective, and could “dangerously” be mistaken for normal individuals. Goddard was a fervent advocate for institutionalization and family removal of the feeble-minded, as well as for mass testing in order to identify them in the general population.

In 1912, German-Jew psychologist William Stern (1871-1931) formulated a more intuitive way to present the results of the Binet-Simon test; the “Intelligence Quotient” (IQ) or ratio of the mental age to biological age multiplied by 100. In the US, many psychologists revised and expanded the Binet-Simon test. Lewis Terman’s (1877-1956) version, the Stanford-Binet test, became the most used. Terman’s public claims to mass administer the test in schools became a reality during the 1920s and 1930s in the US, being so used in interventionist programs and placements (Minton, 1998).

With the coming of World War I, Terman joined a committee of test developers chaired by Robert M. Yerkes (1876-1956), president of the American Psychological Association. This committee developed the Army Alpha and Beta tests administered to 1.75 million people, which determined the placements of army recruits. They hypothesized, in line with their commitment to the inherited mental superiority of whites and the upper class, that these populations will score higher on the tests. The results were congruent with this prediction; however, after the war the results were challenged and subject to much criticism for being biased. With some exceptions (e.g. following the publications of Arthur Jensen in 1969 and The Bell Curve in 1994) psychology has mainly kept away from such debates.

Uses and misuses of intelligence testing
Although rarely mentioned in legislation, intelligence testing had a prominent place in the debate around many eugenic measures, such as segregation and immigration restriction. During the first half of the twentieth century, it was commonplace to advocate for the segregation and compulsory sterilization of the “feeble-minded” in the name of reduction of delinquency, industrial inefficiency, poverty and prostitution. Residential schools and compulsory sterilization laws were introduced in numerous countries. By 1964, of the 60,000 people in the US who had been subject to compulsory sterilization, mental inferiority accounted for more than half of the victims (Mackintosh, 2011a p.23). Feeble-mindedness was a reason for sterilization in 31 out of 33 North American jurisdictions. In theory, eugenic boards relied on IQ testing as an independent tool for measuring intellectual capacity. In practice, however, IQ testing was missing in the diagnosis for about a third of the cases in British Columbia (Heeswijk 1994, Ch. 3) and in Alberta (Keith 2011), although the latter Eugenics Board approved 99% of its 4,785 cases with an average of 13 minutes per case. Today, psychometric tests are a common entry requirement for the police and military bodies, and sometimes are used for employee selection and educational placement in schools and universities. In the DSM V, IQ tests are among the diagnosis criteria for intellectual disability. In courts, they could be a valid factor to determine if the person is capable of informed consent or of parenting.

Conclusion
Historically, psychology’s notion of intelligence is entangled with that of eugenics. The idea that intelligence is a unitary graded psychological trait that can be quantified goes back to Galton, and the development and use of intelligence testing was popularized as a tool to implement eugenic measures. It is hard to deny that there was, and still is, a marked lack of agreement between psychologists on what intelligence is and what it involves: is it fixed or does it change over time? Is it a unitary entity or many? Is it mainly inherited or acquired? Is it connected closely to the peripheries of cognition or to its higher-order elements? Do tests measure functioning or ability? Can conclusions and comparisons be made from tests on groups, or their validity can only on an individual basis? However, the resulting unclarity about what intelligence is has not undermined our past or present practical reliance on intelligence measuring for classifying people.

-Aida Roige

  • Minton, Henry L. (1998). Commentary on: "New methods for the diagnosis of the intellectual level of subnormals" Alfred Binet & Theodore Simon (1905) "The uses of intelligence tests" Lewis M. Terman (1916). Retrieved Jan 20th, 2015, from http://psychclassics.yorku.ca

  • Pickren, W., Rutherford, A. (2010). A History of Modern Psychology in Context. Wiley

  • Plucker, J.A., & Esping, A., Eds. (2014). Human intelligence: Historical influences, current controversies, teaching resources. Retrieved Jan 20th, 2015, from http://www.intelltheory.com.

  • Stern, W. (1914). The Psychological Methods Of Testing Intelligence. Translated from the German by Guy Montrose Whipple. Baltimore, Warwick & York. Page 3.

  • Sternberg, R., & Kauffman, S.B., Eds. (2011). The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.

  • Keith, E. (2011). Human Wreckage from Foreign Lands: A Study of the Ethnic Victims of the Alberta Sterilization Act. Constellations. 2:2 81 – 89.

  • Snyderman, M.; Herrnstein, R. J. (1983). Intelligence tests and the Immigration Act of 1924. American Psychologist, Vol 38(9), 986-995.

  • Trent, J.W.Jr. (1995). Inventing the Feeble Mind: A History of Mental Retardation in the United States. University of California Press.

  • White, S. (2000). Conceptual foundations of IQ testing. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 6 (1), 33-43

  • Mackintosh, N.J. (2011a). IQ and Human Intelligence (2nd ed). Oxford, Oxford University Press.

  • Unknown (1890) Bodies and brains in examinations. The Spectator, April 5th 1890, vol. 64 pp 472-473.

  • Heeswijk , Gail van (1994): "An act respecting sexual sterilization": reasons for enacting and repealing the act. M.A. thesis, University of British Columbia. Available at circle.ubc.ca/handle/2429/5575